Dartmouth-Led Study of Chimpanzees Explores the Early Origins of Human Hand Dexterity

News subtitle

Findings Demonstrate Foraging Advantages of Advanced Manual Prehension

Body

April 22, 2016 – Chimpanzees use manipulative dexterity to evaluate and select figs, a vital resource when preferred foods are scarce, according to a new Dartmouth-led study just published by Interface Focus. The action resembles that of humans shopping for fruits, and the study demonstrates the foraging advantages of opposable fingers and careful manual prehension, or the act of grasping an object with precision. The findings shed new light on the ecological origins of hands with fine motor control, a trait that enabled our early human ancestors to manufacture and use stone tools.

 

Image
3 chimpanzee images
Chimpanzees in Kibale National Park, Uganda, initiate a series of sensory assessments to evaluate the edibility of figs. Sensory assessment entailed from L to R: 1) Visual information; 2) Manual palpation to discern softness (elastic deformation); 3) Incisor evaluation to discern toughness (chewability). All photos by Alain Houle.

“The supreme dexterity of the human hand is unsurpassed among mammals, a fact that is often linked to early tool use,” said lead author Nathaniel J. Dominy, professor of anthropology and adjunct professor of biological sciences at Dartmouth. “A problem is that we know little about the selective pressures that favored the advanced manual skills of chimpanzees and other apes. These skills were the cognitive foundation for the origin of our extraordinary hands, a trait that made all the difference.”

For the study, Dominy and his colleagues observed the foraging behaviors of chimpanzees, black-and-white colobus monkeys, red colobus monkeys and red-tailed monkeys in Kibale National Park, Uganda. The primates depended on figs, and although ripe figs come in a range of colors, many stay green throughout development and every phase can be present on a single tree, making it difficult to discern solely by color, which figs are ripe. To determine if the green figs of Ficus sansibarica are edible, chimpanzees ascend trees and make a series of sensory assessments-- they may look at the fig’s color, smell the fig, manually palpate or touch each fig (using the volar pad of the thumb and lateral side of the index finger) to assess the fruit’s elasticity and/or bite the fig to determine the stiffness of the fruit. Colobus monkeys do not have thumbs and evaluate the ripeness of figs by using their front teeth.

The team examined the spectral, chemical and mechanical properties of figs, which included boring into individual figs to assess the elasticity of the fruit and extracting fig contents to estimate nutritional rewards such as sugar. They observed the non-selection, rejection and ingestion of individual figs, and collected specimens of figs that were: avoided; palpated and rejected; palpated, bitten and rejected; and edible for which less than 50 percent of the fruit was left. Chimpanzees also use their sense of smell to assess individual figs; however, the study was unequipped to capture olfactory volatiles. Based on the sensory data obtained, the team estimated the predictive power that sensory information may have on chimpanzees when estimating the ripeness of figs.

Palpating figs was about four times faster than detaching and then biting the fruit, suggesting that chimpanzees may have a substantial foraging advantage over birds and monkeys, which rely on visual and oral information. The study provides new insight into how chimpanzees exhibit advanced visuomotor control during the foraging process and more broadly, on the evolution of skilled forelimb movements.

Nathaniel J. Dominy, lead author, and professor of anthropology and adjunct professor of biological sciences at Dartmouth, is available for comment at: nathaniel.j.dominy@dartmouth.edu.

Justin D. Yeakel, assistant professor at UC-Merced; Uttam Bhat, graduate fellow at the Santa Fe Institute; Lawrence Ramsden, honorary assistant professor at the University of Hong Kong; Richard W. Wrangham, Ruth B. Moore Professor of Biological Anthropology at Harvard University; and Peter W. Lucas, research associate at the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute, also served as co-authors.

Note to editors: Hi-res images and video are available upon request.